X-bar theory
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
− | The X-bar theory <ref>Chomsky, Noam (1970). Remarks on nominalization. In: R. Jacobs and P. Rosenbaum (eds.) ''Reading in English Transformational Grammar'', 184-221. Waltham: Ginn.</ref> postulates that all human languages share certain structural similarities, including the same underlying syntactic structure, the " | + | The X-bar theory was first proposed by Noam Chomsky (1970). <ref>Chomsky, Noam (1970). Remarks on nominalization. In: R. Jacobs and P. Rosenbaum (eds.) ''Reading in English Transformational Grammar'', 184-221. Waltham: Ginn.</ref> It postulates that all human languages share certain structural similarities, including the same underlying syntactic structure, which is known as the "X-bar". |
== X-bar == | == X-bar == |
Revision as of 19:30, 2 August 2012
The X-bar theory was first proposed by Noam Chomsky (1970). [1] It postulates that all human languages share certain structural similarities, including the same underlying syntactic structure, which is known as the "X-bar".
Contents |
X-bar
The X-bar abstract configuration is depicted in the diagram below:
XP / \ spec XB / \ XB adjt / \ X comp | head
In the above:
- X is the head, the nucleus or the source of the whole syntactic structure, which is actually derived (or projected) out of it.
- comp (i.e., complement) is an internal argument, i.e., a word, phrase or clause which is necessary to the head to complete its meaning (e.g., objects of transitive verbs)
- adjt (i.e., adjunct) is a word, phrase or clause which modifies the head but which is not syntactically required by it (adjuncts are expected to be extranuclear, i.e., removing an adjunct would leave a grammatically well-formed sentence)
- spec (i.e., specifier) is an external argument, i.e., a word, phrase or clause which qualifies (determines) the head
- XB (X-bar) is the general name for any of the intermediate projections derived from X
- XP (X-bar-bar, X-double-bar, X-phrase) is the maximal projection of X.
Constituents
The head, the complement, the specifier and the adjunct are said to be the constituents of the syntactic representation and define the four general universal syntactic roles.
Heads
In the X-bar diagram depicted above, the letter X is used to signify an arbitrary category. Thus, the X may become an N for noun, a V for verb, and so on. In the UNLarium framework, there are eight different types of heads:
- N = nouns and nominals: personal pronouns, demonstrative pronouns, nominalizations, etc
- V = verbs
- J = adjectives
- A = adverbs[2]
- P = prepositions
- D = determiners: articles, demonstrative determiners, possessive determiners, quantifiers
- I = auxiliary verbs
- C = conjunctions
The heads define the nature of the phrase structures, thus:
- N projects a Noun Phrase (NP)
- V projects a Verbal Phrase (VP)
- J projects an Adjective Phrase (JP)
- A projects an Adverbial Phrase (AP)
- P projects a Prepositional Phrase (PP)
- D projects a Determiner Phrase (DP)
- I projects an Inflectional Phrase (IP)
- C projects a Complementizer Phrase (CP)
Noun Phrase | Verbal Phrase | Adjective Phrase | Adverbial Phrase | Prepositional Phrase | Determiner Phrase | Inflectional Phrase | Complementizer Phrase |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
NP / \ spec NB / \ NB adjunct / \ N comp | noun |
VP / \ spec VB / \ VB adjunct / \ V comp | verb |
JP / \ spec JB / \ JB adjunct / \ J comp | adjective |
AP / \ spec AB / \ AB adjunct / \ A comp | adverb |
PP / \ spec PB / \ PB adjunct / \ P comp | preposition |
DP / \ spec DB / \ DB adjunct / \ D comp | determiner |
IP / \ spec IB / \ IB adjunct / \ I comp | auxiliary verb |
CP / \ spec CB / \ CB adjunct / \ C comp | conjunction |
Specifiers
Specifiers are used to narrow the meaning intended by the head. They include:
- articles: the (book), a (book), etc.
- possessive determiners: my (book), your (book), etc.
- demonstrative determiners: this (book), that (book), etc.
- quantifiers: no (answer), every (hour), etc.
- intensifiers (emphasizers, amplifiers, downtoners): very (expensive), quite (well), nearly (under), kind of (like), etc.
- frequency adverbs: always (go), never (go), usually (go), etc.
- negative adverbs: not (go)
Complements
Complements are used to complete the meaning intended by the head. They may be:
- direct objects: (do) something, (give) something
- indirect objects: (laugh at) something, (give to) someone
- complement of deverbals (i.e., nouns deriving from verbs): (construction of) the city, (arrival of) Peter
- complement of adjectives: (loyal) to the queen, (interested) in Chemistry
- complement of adverbs: (contrarily) to popular belief, (independently) from her
- complement of prepositions: (under) the table, (after) today
- complement of conjunctions: (and) Peter, (I don't know if) he'll come
Adjuncts
Adjuncts are used to modify the meaning intended by the head:
- adjectives: beautiful (table)
- manner adverbs: (speak) slowly
- prepositional phrases: (table) of wood
etc.
Examples
NP | VP | JP | PP |
---|---|---|---|
NP / \ / \ / \ / NB / / \ spec head comp | | | D N PP | | | the construction of Babel |
VP | VB / \ / \ / \ VB \ / \ \ head comp adjt | | | V N A | | | (he) bought something yesterday |
JP / \ / \ / \ / JB / / \ spec head comp | | | A J PP | | | (I'm) really interested in Chemistry |
PP | PB / \ head N | | without notice |
Phrases
Specifiers, complements and adjuncts are themselves complex syntactic structures (XPs) which are combined to form the sentence structure:
PHRASE | STRUCTURE | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
XP | ||||
SPEC | XB | |||
XB | ADJT | |||
HEAD | COMP | |||
NP | DP | N | PP | JP,PP |
VP | NP | V | NP,PP | PP,JP,AP |
JP | AP | J | PP | AP,PP |
AP | AP | A | PP | PP,AP |
PP | AP | P | NP,JP,VP | AP |
DP | AP | D | AP | |
IP | AP | I | VP | AP |
CP | AP | C | IP | AP |
Examples
construction | the construction | the fateful construction | the fateful construction of Babel |
---|---|---|---|
NP | NB | N | construction |
NP / \ spec NB | | DP N | | DB construction | D | the |
NP / \ spec NB | / \ DP adjt N | | | DB JP construction | | D JB | | the J | fateful |
NP / \ spec NB | / \ DP adjt NB | | / \ DB JP N comp | | | | D JB construction PP | | | the J PB | / \ fateful P comp | | of NP | NB | N | Babel |
Properties
Branching is binary
A key assumption of X-bar theory is that branching is always binary, if it occurs. This means that specifiers, complements and adjuncts are optional and that there can be as many XBs as necessary:
XP | XB | head |
XP / \ spec XB | head |
XP | XB / \ head comp |
XP | XB / \ head adjt |
XP / \ spec XB / \ head adjt |
XP / \ spec XB / \ XB comp / \ X comp | head |
XP / \ spec XB / \ XB adjt / \ XB comp / \ X comp | head |
etc. |
Order is parametrized
The order of the constituents (specifiers, complements and adjuncts) is subject to language specific parametrization and may vary:
right complement | left complement | right adjunct | left adjunct | |
---|---|---|---|---|
XP | XB / \ head comp |
XP | XB / \ comp head |
XP | XB / \ head adjt |
XP | XB / \ adjt head |
etc. |
Conventions
The following conventions have been adopted in the UNLarium framework. They do not correspond to the current assumptions of the X-bar theory, and derive rather from extralinguistic issues (such as machine-tractability).
CP is the topmost level
In the UNLarium framework, a clause is considered to be an instance of the complementizer phrase (CP), which is the syntactic topmost level.
CP / \ spec CB / \ C IP / \ spec IB / \ I VP
C (i.e., the head of CP) is to be left empty in case of independent clauses (i.e., not introduced by any conjunction).
CP / \ CB / \ IP | [Mary kissed Peter] |
CP / \ CB / \ / \ / \ C IP | | (I know) that [Mary kissed Peter] |
IP represents finite clauses
The Inflectional Phrase (IP) is always the complement of the Complementizer Phrase (CP) and represents finite clauses. In the UNLarium approach, the head of IP is occupied only in case of compound tenses, i.e., by the auxiliary verb carrying information on tense, aspect and mood; in simple tenses, the head of the IP must be left empty.
CP / \ CB / \ IP / \ IB / \ I VP | auxiliary
CP / \ CB / \ IP / \ IB / \ / \ / \ I VP | | [Mary kissed Peter] |
CP / \ CB / \ IP / \ IB / \ / \ / \ I VP | | is [Mary kissing Peter] |
CP / \ CB / \ IP / \ IB / \ / \ / \ I VP | | will [Mary kiss Peter] |
CP / \ CB / \ IP / \ IB / \ / \ / \ I VP | | may [Mary kiss Peter] |
etc. |
Subjects
The subject of a clause is always represented at the position of spec of VP.
VP / \ Mary VB | [died] |
VP / \ Mary VB | [kissed Peter] |
VP / \ Mary VB | [gave a book to John] |
VP / \ Mary VB | [is beautiful] |
etc. |
Objects, Adjuncts and Predicates
There is no structural difference between objects (either direct or indirect) or adjuncts. They are always represented as branches of the intermediate projection. Predicates are represented as complements of copula (linking) verbs.
VB / \ buy NP | [a car] |
VB / \ depend PP | [on me] |
VB / \ go AP | [slowly] |
VB / \ is JP | [beautiful] |
etc. |
Notes
- ↑ Chomsky, Noam (1970). Remarks on nominalization. In: R. Jacobs and P. Rosenbaum (eds.) Reading in English Transformational Grammar, 184-221. Waltham: Ginn.
- ↑ In the X-bar theory, differently from the UNLarium approach, adverbs are subsumed by prepositions and are not considered to be an independent lexical category.